Subscribe
The latest psychology and neuroscience discoveries.
My Account
  • Mental Health
  • Social Psychology
  • Cognitive Science
  • Psychopharmacology
  • Neuroscience
  • About
No Result
View All Result
PsyPost
PsyPost
No Result
View All Result
Home Exclusive Evolutionary Psychology

New study fails to replicate the link between fertility in women and wearing red clothing

by Eric W. Dolan
April 19, 2017
in Evolutionary Psychology, Social Psychology
(Photo credit: EkaterinaJurkova)

(Photo credit: EkaterinaJurkova)

Share on TwitterShare on Facebook
Follow PsyPost on Google News

A new replication study published in the peer-reviewed journal Hormones and Behavior casts doubt on claims that women unconsciously signal their fertility by wearing red clothing.

Previous research had found that women were more likely to wear red or pink at peak fertility. In other female mammals, such as chimpanzees, red skin coloration is used to advertise the fertile window in an observable manner (estrus). Humans do not display observable physical changes associated with ovulation, but it was thought women may still be using the color red to attract mates at peak fertility.

The new replication study tested the association between ovarian hormones and wearing red clothing using an ethnically diverse sample of 164 women who were 18 to 36 years old. But the researchers found no compelling evidence that women who were more fertile were more likely to wear red than another color of clothing.

There was a weak relationship between fertility and wearing red only among 18– to 22-year-old women, but this relationship could be a statistical artifact. The relationship only existed when all non-red clothing was grouped into a single category. “We did not find compelling evidence of this effect when using other statistical techniques,” the study concludes.

PsyPost interviewed the study’s corresponding author, Khandis R. Blake of the University of New South Wales, about the research. Read her responses below:

PsyPost: Why were you interested in this topic?

Blake: I am interested in female intrasexual competition, and the hypothesis that women choose to wear red clothing when they are fertile has potentially profound implications for understanding the evolution of female mating strategies. We were also in a lucky position in that the data we analyzed was collected as part of a larger study and for another purpose. We had photographs of women’s clothing at two time points and corresponding estradiol and progesterone values already, so this meant it was easy for us to see if we could replicate recent findings.

What should the average person take away from your study?

Past work has shown that women’s tendency to wear red clothing tracks their potential fertility, including their estradiol to progesterone ratio. The E:P ratio is a hormonal marker of fertility in the menstrual cycle, where high values indicate a greater likelihood of conception from intercourse. We show that the relationship between the estradiol to progesterone ratio and the likelihood of wearing red only arises for women who are very young (18–22 years old). Because researchers have used the relationship between potential fertility and red clothing to claim that women exhibit estrus, this is an important finding. If only a subgroup of fertile women wear more red when they are fertile, and we have no reason to doubt their ability to conceive (which we didn’t)—then red clothing is probably not a cue of human ovulation.

The other, perhaps more important thing to take away is that we could only replicate the relationship between the E:P ratio and red clothing when using a specific kind of statistical technique. This technique involved grouping all possible color combinations par red (and pink) and comparing this amalgamated group to red. This is the technique other researchers who have found the red-fertility effect have used. The technique doesn’t distinguish between the frequency of other color combinations, and instead groups everything that isn’t red as one color (“not red”) and compares this to red (it’s a binary technique). In everyday life, the choice between choosing clothing colors is probably more complex than this grouping allows. Testing the hypothesis that women choose to wear one color when E:P ratios are high should also ideally include an investigation of all possible color choices as well.

In our study, we undertook this approach. We compared binary and multinomial analyses to see whether we could replicate the red-fertility effect using different techniques. When we investigated the association between the E:P ratio and red clothing when differentiating amongst the other colors worn by the sample (a multinomial technique), no compelling relationship between the variables arose. What does this mean? Together, these results show that the red-fertility effect only arises under particular statistical conditions and only for a subgroup of women (very young women).

Are there any major caveats? What questions still need to be addressed?

One unresolved question is why there was a positive association between E:P ratios and the probability of wearing red for younger women (albeit one we couldn’t replicate using multinomial techniques). It seems as if red clothing may be a mate attraction tactic employed by young potentially fertile women in some conditions, but future research is needed to replicate this effect and identify when and for whom the effect applies. My hunch is that the use of red clothing amongst younger, fertile women—if the effect replicates—is probably related to experience effects. Future work will tell.

Is there anything else you would like to add?

Signaling sexual proceptivity or enhancing attractiveness are just two of many possible reasons for women’s clothing color choices. The role of other factors that may potentially influence clothing color choice—for example, mood, clothing color availability, or stable personal preferences for particular clothing colors—have been neglected by empirical research. Future studies could investigate whether fertile women prefer ornamentation that captures visual attention rather than red ornamentation per se, or whether other signals of sexual proceptivity (e.g., sexualized clothing) are affected by ovarian hormones and mating-relevant goals. Women’s choice to wear red clothing is probably more nuanced than previously theorized.

The study, “No compelling positive association between ovarian hormones and wearing red clothing when using multinomial analyses“, was also co-authored by Barnaby J.W. Dixson, Siobhan M. O’Dean, and Thomas F. Denson.

TweetSendScanShareSendPinShareShareShareShareShare

RELATED

Psychopathic tendencies may be associated with specific hormonal patterns
Psychopathy

Psychopathic tendencies may be associated with specific hormonal patterns

July 10, 2025

What if the roots of psychopathy could be traced in our blood? New research is looking beyond psychology and into our endocrine system. A new study suggests hormones like cortisol and testosterone may hold important clues to a person's manipulative and impulsive tendencies.

Read moreDetails
Testosterone shifts political preferences in weakly affiliated Democratic men, study finds
Political Psychology

Testosterone shifts political preferences in weakly affiliated Democratic men, study finds

July 10, 2025

What if the key to swaying a swing voter lies in their biology? New research found giving testosterone to weakly affiliated Democratic men made them less loyal to their party and more open to Republicans, revealing a potential hormonal link to political persuasion.

Read moreDetails
Bullshit is deemed more credible if attributed to a scientist, compared to a spiritual guru
Social Psychology

Scientists who relocate more often start Nobel research up to two years earlier

July 8, 2025

A new study of Nobel Prize winners suggests that scientists who change locations or work in multiple places tend to begin their groundbreaking research earlier, highlighting how exposure to diverse environments may help spark innovative, high-impact ideas.

Read moreDetails
The most popular dementia videos on TikTok tend to have the lowest quality, study find
Addiction

People with short-video addiction show altered brain responses during decision-making

July 8, 2025

People who frequently use short-video apps like TikTok may show reduced loss sensitivity and impulsive decision-making, according to a new neuroimaging study that links addictive use patterns to changes in brain activity during risky choices.

Read moreDetails
People with psychopathic traits fail to learn from painful outcomes
Narcissism

National narcissism linked to emotional impairments and dehumanization, new study finds

July 7, 2025

A new study suggests that people who see their nation as uniquely important often struggle with recognizing emotions and experience more anger and contempt—factors that may help explain why they’re more likely to dehumanize both outsiders and fellow citizens.

Read moreDetails
Anxious and avoidant attachment are elevated among individuals with eating disorders
Developmental Psychology

Personality may be a key factor connecting negative parenting experiences to adult challenges

July 7, 2025

New research finds a link between how adults recall being parented and their current life difficulties. These challenges may be partly explained by personality traits, such as lower conscientiousness, that are connected to early family environments and adult well-being.

Read moreDetails
Loneliness predicts an increase in TV viewing for older women, but not for men
Sexism

New study finds link between sexism and denial of male victimhood in relationships

July 6, 2025

New research shows that many people endorse myths that minimize abuse against men in relationships. These myths are closely tied to sexist attitudes about masculinity, gender roles, and who is believed to be a “real” victim of violence.

Read moreDetails
Fascinating study reveals how Trump’s moral rhetoric diverges from common Republican language
Donald Trump

Viral AI-images highlight how Trump engages in “victimcould,” scholar argues

July 6, 2025

How can one of the world's most powerful men also be its biggest victim? A new paper argues it’s a political strategy based on hypothetical, not actual, harm—a concept the author calls “victimcould” used to justify present-day aggression.

Read moreDetails

SUBSCRIBE

Go Ad-Free! Click here to subscribe to PsyPost and support independent science journalism!

STAY CONNECTED

LATEST

Dementia: Your lifetime risk may be far greater than previously thought

Psychopathic tendencies may be associated with specific hormonal patterns

Scientists use deep learning to uncover hidden motor signs of neurodivergence

Study finds “Anxious Mondays” linked to long-term stress and heart health risks in older adults

Adults treated with psychostimulants for ADHD show increased brain surface complexity, study finds

Is humor inherited? Twin study suggests the ability to be funny may not run in the family

Testosterone shifts political preferences in weakly affiliated Democratic men, study finds

Can sunshine make you happier? A massive study offers a surprising answer

         
       
  • Contact us
  • Privacy policy
  • Terms and Conditions
[Do not sell my information]

Welcome Back!

Login to your account below

Forgotten Password?

Retrieve your password

Please enter your username or email address to reset your password.

Log In

Add New Playlist

Subscribe
  • My Account
  • Cognitive Science Research
  • Mental Health Research
  • Social Psychology Research
  • Drug Research
  • Relationship Research
  • About PsyPost
  • Contact
  • Privacy Policy